Volume 6, Issue 1 (Continuously Updated 2023)                   Func Disabil J 2023, 6(1): 0-0 | Back to browse issues page


XML Print


Download citation:
BibTeX | RIS | EndNote | Medlars | ProCite | Reference Manager | RefWorks
Send citation to:

Almasi M, Mirzajani A, Abolghasemi J, Rahmani S. Examining the Relationship Between the Dominant Eye and the Dominant Hand. Func Disabil J 2023; 6 (1) : 233.1
URL: http://fdj.iums.ac.ir/article-1-196-en.html
1- Department of Optometry, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran.
2- Department of Optometry, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran. , mirzajani.a@iums.ac.ir
3- Department of Biostatistics, School of Health, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran.
4- Department of Optometry, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran.
Full-Text [PDF 1006 kb]   (133 Downloads)     |   Abstract (HTML)  (358 Views)
Full-Text:   (93 Views)
Introduction
Although the human anatomy is symmetrically arranged around a central vertical axis, most people use one side of their body with greater frequency, ease, and skill than the other [1]. It is also the case with the eyes. Porta first proposed ocular dominance (OD) in 1953 [2]. Ocular dominance refers to the tendency of the visual system to prefer the processing of input signals from one eye to the other eye [3]. The concept of ocular dominance has been studied for many years in optometry, ophthalmology, and psychology. Clinically, ocular dominance has been used for a variety of applications, including monovision contact lens wear, cataract surgery, sports performance, military marksmanship, and education and learning disorders [4]. Depending on the operational definition and specific measurements, ocular dominance could be classified into sighting, motor, and sensory dominance. Sighting dominance refers to the preferential use of one eye over the fellow eye in fixating on a target. In motor dominance, the dominant eye is less likely to lose fixation at the near point of convergence, and the state of the extraocular muscles with their innervational patterns may play a role in determining motor dominance. Sensory dominance occurs when the perception of a stimulus presented to one eye dominates over the other in retinal rivalry conditions [3].
Qiu et al [5] showed that when OD was determined using the hole-in-the-card test, right OD was present in 75.7% of subjects, and left OD was present in 24.3%. The worth 4-dot (sensory) test indicated right OD in 40.7%, left OD in 39.3%, and 20.0% had undetermined ocular dominance. Another study [1] showed that using the hole-in-card test and +1.50 to blur (sensory) test, 71% of subjects with the hole-in-card test and 54% of subjects with the +1.50 to blur test had right-eye dominance, which had a statistically significant difference.
In this study, the relationship between hand-eye dominance is investigated. According to the definition, the dominant hand performs better, faster, and more accurately than the other [6]. Handedness is a multidimensional motor function that identifies the hand one prefers to use for a variety of unimanual tasks (i.e. preference) and the ability to perform more effectively with one hand (i.e. performance) [7]. Therefore, preference and performance dimensions should be considered in determining the handedness to choose it more accurately. Hand dominance is strong and pure when the preference and performance in one hand are significantly preferable to the other [8]. Based on studies [9, 10], cultural and biological factors may influence the dominant hand. In the early twentieth century, significant pressures were found to switch the writing hand from left to right to avoid the religious and social stigmas of being left-handed. It was believed that left-handed individuals were at a physical and psychological disadvantage compared to right-handers. In addition to forced hand switching, inherent environmental constraints exist that a left-handed individual must adapt to living in a world designed for right-handers. In addition to these pressures, due to the design of devices for right-handers, left-handers people have to adapt and get used to this type of design, which, in turn, can affect the accurate recognition of whether people are left-handed or right-handed [11]. As a result, environmental factors can affect the preference and performance dominance of the hand, and it is impossible to determine the dominant hand by performing one test that evaluates only one of the dimensions. Because a person may have left-hand dominance, and the preference is changed to the right hand due to parental compulsion, the left hand still performs better. Only a few studies have paid attention to the effect of these factors, which may affect the results of studies. In past classifications, typically, hand dominance was determined unidimensionally (such as a person’s self-report of their dominant hand). Recently, multidimensional hand dominance has been considered, as its one-dimensionality seemed ambiguous. According to the study conducted by Corey et al [12], if considered alone, performance-based measurements are not predictors of preferred hand. Combining the questionnaires and performance test results can be the best predictor of the preferred hand. Thus, these results show that the use of several different methods can predict the dominant hand more accurately. Also, regarding eye dominance, different methods can be used to determine the dominant eye. Therefore, investigating the effect of the method of determining the dominance in the eye and its relationship with the dominance of the hand can bring new results. Therefore, this study aims to obtain a more accurate and comprehensive laterality of them, considering all the dimensions of hand and eye dominance, according to the available facilities and the results of past studies.

Materials and Methods
This research was conducted by convenience sampling method in Qom City, Iran in 2022. People without physical defects in the upper limbs and with normal vision (at least visual acuity of 20/20 measured by the Snellen e-chart) between the ages of 17 and 35 years whom we got access to, were invited to participate in the study if they had the conditions to enter the research. In this study, information, such as a history of eye surgery, and history of eye trauma, was collected by asking questions in the form of an interview. The person’s refractive error was obtained objectively by auto refractometer Topcon model AR 8000 and then subjectively with an end-point of maximum plus for better distance visual acuity. With the best correction, the subject’s visual acuity was measured with a Snellen e-chart placed at 6 meters from the participant. The amount of eye deviation was measured first at a distance of 6 meters and then at a distance of 40 cm using an accommodative target with the alternate prism cover test. Stereopsis was measured with the Titmus Fly test. Sighting ocular dominance was determined with the hole-in-card test (to find out which eye was used for fixation), motor ocular dominance with the near-point-of-convergence (NPC) test (to find which eye is less likely to lose fixation at the near point of convergence), and sensory ocular dominance with +1.50 to blur test (to find out which eye feels more blur by adding +1.50 sphere to the best optical correction).
To determine the performance dominance of the hand, the finger-tapping test was used. We used the finger-tapping test software, version 3.5 (Sybu Data) that can be installed on the mobile phone. In this method, the participant was asked to tap the index finger in the box specified in the software as fast as possible for ten s while his/her palm was on the table. This task was repeated three times for each hand and the average tap number was recorded. The hand with the higher average tap number was considered the dominant performance hand. Preference for hand dominance was also determined by asking several questions in the form of an interview, and the preferred hand for writing, throwing, using a spoon, brushing, and combing was questioned. Finally, according to the results of the tests, the dominant hand and dominant eye were determined and recorded.

Statistical analysis
All data were analyzed using SPSS software, version 27 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL). The chi-square test has been used to evaluate the relationship between variables. In all statistical tests, P<0.05 was considered significant.

Results
The subject group (74 people, mean age 22.7±2.3 years) included 36 men (48.6%) (mean age 22.9±2.3 years) and 38 women (51.4%) (mean age 22.6±2.3 years). Age distributions of the genders were not statistically significantly different (P=0.597).
Table 1 presents the OD outcomes recorded with the three dominance test formats across the study group. As can be seen, 66% (49 of 74) of individuals were identified as right-eye dominant using the sighting test, 51% (38 of 74) of persons were right-eye dominant with the sensory test, and 57% (42 of 74) of persons were right-eye dominant with the motor test. Also, with the sighting test, all participants had clear ocular dominance, while 14 participants (18.91%) had undetermined OD with motor and sensory tests. This represents a statistically significantly different outcome between the sighting and motor tests (P=0.000), between sighting and sensory tests (P=0.003), and between motor and sensory tests (P=0.003).



In examining the pure dominant eye, a significant change was observed (Table 2) so that only 35% (26 of 74) of subjects were pure right-eyed (all three sighting, motor, and sensory dominance were present in the right eye simultaneously), and 53% (39 of 74) of persons all three dimensions of OD were not present in one eye simultaneously, in other words, they did not have pure ocular dominance. 



Table 3 presents the distribution of the pure dominant hand (in other words, both dimensions of performance dominance and preference dominance are in the same hand). When the pure dominant hand was analyzed, 80% (59 of 74) of subjects were right-handed.



Table 4 presents the results of the two hand dominance format tests. According to the results, most participants were right-handed both in the preference test (84%, 62 of 74) and in the performance test (85%, 63 of 74). Based on these results, a significant non-random relationship is observed between preference and performance tests of hand dominance (P=0.003).



Table 5 presents the frequency distribution and comparison of net dominant hand and eye, and based on that, out of a total of 59 people whose right hand was dominant, 23 people (39.0%) had the right eye, and 4 people (6.8%) had the left eye. Also, among the right-handed people, 32 people (54.2%) did not have a specific dominant eye; in other words, all 3 dimensions of eye dominance were not observed in one eye. Also, out of 4 people whose left hand was dominant, 1 person (0.25%) had the right eye, and 3 person (0.75%) had the left eye. Of the 11 people who did not have a clear dominant hand, none were right-eyed, but either their left eye was dominant (36.4%), or they did not have a clear eye dominance in the eye (63.6%). Based on statistical analysis, a significant non-random relationship is observed between pure dominant hand and eye dominance (P=0.000). It is worth noting that the pure dominant hand (0.813) and pure dominant eye (P=0.126) are independent of gender.




Discussion
Based on the results of this study, a non-random relationship is observed between the pure dominant eye and the pure dominant hand. In other words, the pure hand and the pure eye dominance are not independent. However, in a study conducted by Jagadamba [13], it was pointed out that no significant relationship is observed between the dominant hand and the eye. The relationship between sighting dominance and the preferred dominant hand has been investigated in that study. However, if the relationship between these two cases is considered according to the data in the present study, in that case, a significant non-random relationship is observed between sighting dominance and the preferred dominant hand (P=0.034). With simple definitions regarding the dominance of eye and hand, that is, if the dominant eye, observational eye, and the dominant hand, handwriting are considered, the relationship between the eye and the dominant hand becomes significant (P=0.040).
The current study found that when each of the different dimensions of eye dominance is considered alone, the frequency of right eyes is higher in all three dimensions. Dominant eye in few people is relatively undetermined, which can be found in various studies [1, 5, 13]. Nevertheless, the result is very different when all dimensions are considered together, and we define the dominant eye as having all 3 dimensions of eye dominance in that eye. It is observed that most people (52.7%) have all the dimensions of dominance not in one eye, and do not have a pure dominant eye. The results show a significant relationship between the results of all three tests used to determine eye dominance in this study. Now the question may arise that in clinical conditions when one-sided eye treatment is to be done, which of the dimensions of eye dominance is a better indicator of the dominant eye? Given that the human visual system is usually binocular, are tests that allow some degree of binocular vision to be maintained during testing desirable? Even in many people with the normal visual system in sensory and motor tests, no clear dominant eye is observed; clinical decision-making based on the results of these tests may not be a correct basis.
In the current study, the interview method was used to determine the preferred dominant hand, so it is possible that some people refused to give real answers and gave unrealistic answers. Also, the large number of experiments and tests caused the participant’s fatigue and errors in the results of the tests. This study was conducted in the age range of 19-29 years. Therefore, it cannot be generalized to the whole of society.

Ethical Considerations

Compliance with ethical guidelines

The Ethics Committee of the Iran University of Medical Sciences approved this study (Code: IR.IUMS. REC.1400.329) .

Funding
The paper was extracted from the master' s thesis of the Mohammad Almasi, approved by Department of Optometry, School of Rehabilitation Sciences, Iran University of Medical Sciences.

Authors' contributions
Conceptualization: Saeed Rhmani, Ali Mirzajani and Mohammad Almasi; Methodology and writing the original draft: Mohammad Almasi and Ali Mirzajani; Investigation: Mohammad Almasi; Data analysis: Mohammad Almasi and Jamileh Abolghasemi; Review and editing: All authors; Supervision: Ali Mirzajani.

Conflict of interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments
The authors appreciate the cooperation of all participants who have collaborated in this research.



References
  1. Pointer JS. Sighting versus sensory ocular dominance. J Optom. 2012; 5(2):52-5. [DOI:10.1016/j.optom.2012.03.001] [PMCID]
  2. Yuan L, Wan B, Bao Y. Association between ocular dominance and refraction in myopic subjects. Chinese J Ophthalmol. 2020; 56(9):693-8. [DO:10.3760/cma.j.cn112142-20200206-00050]
  3. Jiang F, Chen Z, Bi H, Ekure E, Su B, Wu H, et al. Association between ocular sensory dominance and refractive error asymmetry. PLoS One. 2015; 10(8):e0136222. [DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0136222] [PMID] [PMCID]
  4. Dalton K, Guillon M, Naroo SA. Ocular dominance and handedness in golf putting. Optom Vis Sci. 2015; 92(10):968-75 [DOI:10.1097/OPX.0000000000000690] [PMID]
  5. Qiu JK, Zhang Sh, Wang Z. Comparison of Worth 4-dot test and hole-in-the-card test for the detection of the dominant eye under habitual and best refractive correction. Hong Kong J Ophthalmol. 2006; 10(1):11-4. [Link]
  6. Xiaoli Guo, Yuanyuan Lyu, Bekrater-Bodmann R, Flor H, Tong Sh. Handedness change after dominant side amputation: Evaluation from a hand laterality judgment task. Annu Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2015; 2015:8002-5. [DOI:10.1109/EMBC.2015.7320249] [PMID]
  7. Scharoun SM, Bryden PJ. Is strength of handedness reliable over repeated testing? An examination of typical development and autism spectrum disorder. Front Psychol. 2015; 6:17. [DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00017] [PMID] [PMCID]
  8. Rigal RA. Which handedness: Preference or performance? Percept Mot Skills. 1992; 75(3 Pt 1):851-66. [DOI:10.2466/pms.1992.75.3.851] [PMID]
  9. Galobardes B, Bernstein MS, Morabia A. The association between switching hand preference and the declining prevalence of left-handedness with age. Am J Public Health. 1999; 89(12):1873-5. [DOI:10.2105/AJPH.89.12.1873] [PMID] [PMCID]
  10. Porac C, Buller T. Overt attempts to change hand preference: A study of group and individual characteristics. Can J Psychol. 1990; 44(4):512-21. [DOI:10.1037/h0084268] [PMID]
  11. Adamo DE, Taufiq A. Establishing hand preference: Why does it matter? Hand (N Y). 2011; 6(3):295-303. [DOI:10.1007/s11552-011-9324-x] [PMID] [PMCID]
  12. Corey DM, Hurley MM, Foundas AL. Right and left handedness defined: A multivariate approach using hand preference and hand performance measures. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 2001; 14(3):144-52. [PMID]
  13. Aswathappa J, Kutty K, Annamalai N. Relationship between handedness and ocular dominance in healthy young adults – A study. Int J Pharm Biomed Res. 2011; 2(2):76-8. [Link]
Type of Study: Research | Subject: Optometry
Received: 2023/01/17 | Accepted: 2023/11/5 | Published: 2023/02/20

Add your comments about this article : Your username or Email:
CAPTCHA

Rights and permissions
Creative Commons License This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.

 

Designed & Developed by : Yektaweb